INTRODUCTION

Smoking remains a primary cause of morbidity and mortality and constitutes one of the most significant public health challenges of our time1. It can lead to a myriad of adverse health consequences, including cardiovascular disease, chronic respiratory health problems, and many cancers, to mention a few. The use of cigarettes and other tobacco products account for more than 6 million deaths annually worldwide, with smoking affecting developed and developing populations alike2.

One of the most alarming findings concerning the occurrence of smoking initiation in early adolescence is that more than 90% of adult smokers begin smoking at an age younger than 18 years3. Early experimentation has been associated with a higher risk of nicotine dependence, leading to nicotine addiction that extends into adulthood4. Given the above observations, the implementation of tobacco use prevention programs in schools are of primary importance and require overall prioritization. Efforts should focus on early intervention, with tobacco control initiatives targeting the younger generation5.

Preventative programs in schools display broad heterogeneity, focusing on traditional informational booklet distribution and delivery of class-based educational sessions6 to contemporary approaches that focus on the cultivation of personal skills that challenge negative smoking influences7.

Examples of such programs include the Social Psychological Deterrents of Smoking in Schools Project8, The North Karelia Youth Project9, the Minnesota Smoking Prevention Program10, the Project SHOUT (Students Helping Others Understand Tobacco)11, the Tobacco and Alcohol Prevention Program (TAPP)12, the Life Skills Training (LST) project13, the Project Towards No Tobacco Use (Project TNT)14, the Stay Away from Tobacco (SAFT) program15, and the Drug Abuse Resistance Education (DARE) program16.

While the content of preventative programs is important, delivery and implementation methods retain equal significance, influencing the effectiveness of interventions. Experiential learning is characterized by a student-centered educational approach that enables active student participation in teacher-led, transformational activities17. In essence, it utilizes previously acquired experiences, such as daily encounters and real-life scenarios, to influence personal attitude change. Strategies implemented in class may pivot around active dialogue, role-playing, productive debate, and peer-led teaching. Experiential learning has been shown to improve participation, enhance educational experiences and promote long-lasting enlightenment18,19. Unfortunately, few experiential learning programs have been previously reported in general health or smoking prevention literature, despite promising efficacy.

The proposed and widely used framework of experiential learning, called 4-H, reflects an active educational approach. This approach facilitates the participation and collaboration of students and adults and comprises skill-learning exercises and peer-led sessions20. One of the few experiential learning programs reported in wider literature was by Onion and Bartzokas21 who employed an interactive educational strategy to increase participation. Results demonstrated an enduring attitude change among primary care physicians in complying with evidence-based guidelines for optimal infection treatment22. Experiential learning programs on school-centered tobacco control are less frequently reported. Norman et al.23 tried the value of a theory-based experiential intervention in encouraging smoking cessation. They found that a smoking intervention was more effective than control in prompting immediate motivation to quit22,23.

Previous, successful tobacco control programs were considered when creating this individualized, experiential learning program for middle- and high-school students. We designed a multi-faceted program, incorporating characteristics from traditional programs, such as education about the harmful effects of smoking and newer programs that focus on developing associated personal and social skills. We adopted an interactive, experiential-learning process, assuming different elements from ancient Greek literature and mythology.

Our study aimed to implement and evaluate the effectiveness of a smoking prevention program in facilitating knowledge acquisition, forging healthy attitudes against smoking and decreasing intention to smoke among middle- and high-school students in Athens, Greece.

METHODS

The study was conducted during the 2016–2017 academic year among middle-school students in Athens, Greece. Permission to implement the study was obtained from the Greek Ministry of Education, and ethical approval was granted by the Bioethics Committee of Evangelismos Hospital. The study was supervised by faculty members of the first ICU clinic of Evangelismos Hospital, National and Kapodistrian University of Athens (16/6/2016, Protocol Number 131).

Study participants

The study was fulfilled in five selected middle schools. Students from 1st to 3rd grade in middle-school were invited to participate in intervention and control groups. The final study group was a convenient sample obtained from participating middle schools. Among 407 students from different classes that were randomly allocated to intervention (206) or the control group (201), 56 were excluded since there was no parental approval, were absent once, or completed incorrectly the questionnaires. Finally, 170 students were enrolled in the control group and 181 in the intervention group.

The experiential tobacco control intervention took place in school classrooms of 20–25 students during their everyday program and lasted for two school hours plus the in-between break. Students from both groups completed the questionnaires twice. Once before and secondly 3 months after the intervention. Data collection lasted about seven months (the academic year 2016–2017) at five public schools of the northwestern suburbs of Athens after the protocol was presented and approved by each school director.

Interventions

The Theory of Reasoned Action and Planned Behavior, accepted as a fundamental psychological concept in the prediction of health-related behavior, formed the overarching framework of our study.

We wished to design and implement an innovative and attractive experiential learning prevention program for adolescent students, incorporating new elements that could meet their developmental needs without provoking resistance from their part. We designed and configured our intervention to give the best result in three axes: the provision of knowledge, the recognition of social influences and the development of personal resistance skills, including not accepting cigarettes from peers. The first axis consisted of a presentation on respiratory health, nicotine dependence and the immediate adverse effects of smoking. The other two experiential learning axes were achieved interactively through an interdisciplinary approach. Our educational material consisted of excerpts from the large repository of the ancient and classical Greek literature (Homeric epics, works by Aristotle, Herodotus, Plutarch), the Aesop Fables as well as selections from modern Greek poetry, where archetypal symbols were decoded and applied in tobacco control paradigms. We utilized their didactic character, their reference to human values, their symbolic content, and their heroes’ vivid figures in our intervention, including role-playing to build personal-centered social skills (resistance to peer pressure, building self-efficacy etc.), and instructor-led, pre-designed, student debates. For example, the Aesop Fable ‘Fox Without Tail’ was used in order to develop skills of resistance to peer pressure and social stereotypes, the ancient Greek myth ‘Hercules at the Crossroads: Vice and Virtue’ aimed at developing proper judgement, the adventures of Odysseus on the island of Aeolus and the island of the Sirens, pointed to the irreversible nature of choices such as the concept of addiction. Cavafy’s poems ‘The windows’ and ‘Walls’ that negotiate personal and inner freedom, free will and free choice were used in order to highlight the loss of autonomy that occurs with the onset of smoking.

Data collection

Change in knowledge, attitude, and intention to smoke, was evaluated before and after the intervention. A pre-post test questionnaire was administered at baseline and at follow-up at 3 months, respectively. Data were collected through an anonymous self-administered questionnaire, distributed to all students in both groups. The questionnaire covered demographic information and student smoking behavior, in addition to knowledge, attitudes, and intention to smoke. The final questionnaire integrated items from the Global Youth Tobacco Survey (GYTS - Standard Core Questionnaire)24, the Theory of Reasoned Action and Planned Behavior questionnaire25, and the questionnaire from another study26.

Statistical analysis

Data were entered into a computerized database and analyzed using the Statistical Package for Social Sciences (IBM SPSS V.26). Frequencies and percentages, and means and standard deviations were calculated for descriptive purposes. A chi-squared test was used for categorical variables, while a t-test and Analysis of Variance (ANOVA) were used for continuous variables. To discretely assess students’ knowledge, attitudes and intention toward smoking, a total score was calculated for each outcome. For corresponding questions: positive answers were tabulated as 1, negative answers as -1, and neutral answers as 0. The score of the knowledge and attitudes scale takes values from -16 (incomplete knowledge and attitudes) to +16 (full knowledge and attitudes). Attitudes scores ranged from -9 to +9, respectively. Regression analysis was employed to examine the intervention effect size of knowledge and attitudes, as well as the impact of other factors such as age, gender, and baseline scores on pre- and post-intervention scores. Specifically, the difference of the total score of pre- and post-measurement on knowledge (first model) and attitudes (second model) was the dependent variable of each linear regression analysis, respectively. Independent variables were the students’ age depicted by the grade, gender, group (intervention or control) and the pre-measurement total score of knowledge and attitudes, respectively for the above models. A two-tailed p-value of ≤0.05 was considered statistically significant.

RESULTS

A total of 351 students participated in our study, with 181 (51.6%) in the intervention group and 170 (48.4%) in the control group. The mean age of the participants was 13.0 years (SD=0.96) years; 12.8 years (SD=0.9) in the intervention and 13.3 years (SD=0.95) in the control group (p<0.001). The majority of the participants were females (191; 54.5%), and the distribution of middle-school students across grades was 202 (57.5%) in Grade 1, 68 (19.4%) in Grade 2, and 81 (23.1%) in Grade 3. There was a significant difference in age distribution between the intervention and control groups (p<0.001) depicted by the school grade. Two out of three (69%) of first grade students were in the intervention group versus 45.3% in the control group. There were more students in second and third grade in the control group (55.7%) versus 31% of the intervention group. Non-smoking adolescents were at the same level in both groups (81.8%, p=0.91). These results are described in more details in Table 1. Regarding family smoking, there was no statistically significant difference between intervention and control group in all categories depicted in Table 1. For none, mother, father, both, other member and ex-smoking in the intervention group the results were (50.6, 54.0, 53.8, 53.8, 56.1, 39.5 and 44.8%) and in the control group they were (49.4, 46.0, 46.2, 43.8, 60.5 and 55.2%). Regarding friends’ smoking rates, there was no statistically significant difference between the intervention and the control group (p=0.150).

Table 1

Demographic and other characteristics of 351 students

CharacteristicsTotal (N=351) n (%)Intervention (n=170) n (%)Control (n=181) n (%)p
Mean age, mean ± SD13.0±0.9612.76±0.9113.26±0.95<0.001
Gender
Male16073 (42.9)87 (48.1)0.33
Female19197 (57.1)94 (51.9)
Grade
1st202125 (69.0)77 (45.3)
2nd6830 (16.6)38 (22.4)<0.001
3rd8126 (14.4)55 (32.3)
Smoking status
No287148 (81.8)139 (81.8)
Tried4926 (14.3)23 (13.5)0.91
More than once157 (3.9)8 (4.7)
Family smoking
None17287 (50.58)85 (49.42)0.717
Mother11361 (53.98)52 (46.02)0.552
Father11964 (53.78)55 (46.22)0.533
Both5732 (56.14)25 (43.86)0.450
Other member3815 (39.47)23 (60.53)0.114
Ex-smoking6730 (44.77)37 (55.23)0.216
Friends’ smoking
None273140 (77.3)133 (78.2)
Best friend65 (2.8)1 (0.6)
Some6836 (19.9)32 (18.8)0.150
Most10 (0)1 (0.6)
All30 (0)3 (1.8)

A statistically significant improvement in knowledge acquisition in the intervention group was observed (prescore 5.6±2.1, postscore 7.7±1.7, p<0.001). In contrast, there was a statistically significant decrease in knowledge in the control group (prescore 6.5±1.7, postscore 6.1±1.8, p=0.005). Gender analysis revealed the above statistically significant improvement in knowledge in the intervention group. In boys, the prescore was 5.6±2.2, whilst the postscore was 7.7±1.3 (p<0.001). In girls, similar results were found (Table 2).

Table 2

Outcome measures pre- and post-intervention in both student groups (N=351)

OutcomesIntervention (n=170)
Control (n=181)
prepostpprepostp
Knowledge on adverse effects of smoking
Total score5.6±2.17.7±1.7<0.0016.5±1.76.1±1.80.005
Boys5.6±2.27.7±1.3<0.0016.6±1.86.2±1.90.040
Girls5.6±2.67.7±1.1<0.0016.5±1.86.2±1.80.123
1st Grade5.8±2.17.6±1.3<0.0016.4±1.76.6±1.90.292
2nd Grade5.0±2.08.0±0.8<0.0016.5±1.85.8±1.7<0.001
3rd Grade5.2±2.27.8±1.1<0.0016.7±1.16.6±1.80.597
Attitudes against smoking
Total score5.3±2.77.1±2.1<0.0016.0±2.55.5±3.00.014
Boys4.9±2.97.1±1.9<0.0015.2±2.44.5±3.20.019
Girls5.7±2.67.1±2.4<0.0016.6±2.46.3±2.70.264
1st Grade5.5±2.77.3±2.3<0.0016.4±2.35.6±3.2<0.001
2nd Grade4.9±3.16.7±2.0<0.0015.7±2.95.9±2.90.521
3rd Grade4.6±2.46.8±1.7<0.0015.7±2.55.3±3.10.177
Intention to smoke in the next 12 months*
Unlikely147 (81.2)171 (94.4)140 (82.4)128 (75.3)
Likely30 (16.6)9 (5.0)*23 (13.5)35 (20.6)*
Very likely4 (2.2)1 (0.6)7 (4.1)7 (4.1)

Numbers are given as either mean ± standard deviation or n (%).

* There was a statistically significant difference between intervention and control group with respect to post intervention intention to smoke (p<0.001).

In all grades, a statistically significant improvement in knowledge level was found in the intervention group (p<0.001). The highest increase depicted in the 2nd grade, in which the prescore was 5±2 and the postscore was 8±0.8. In this grade was found a slight decrease in postscore (5.8±1.7) compared to prescore (6.5±1.8) in the control group, which was statistically significant (p<0.001). Similar results were found at pre- and post-intervention scores (Table 2), in grades 1st and 3rd, whilst in the control group in 1st grade, the prescore was 6.4±1.7 and the postscore was 6.6±1.9 (p=0.292). In the 3rd grade, the prescore was 6.7±1.1 and the postscore was 6.6±1.8 (p=0.597).

The attitudes total postscore was higher in the intervention group (7.1±2.1) compared to the prescore (5.3±2.7) (p<0.001). In the control group, there was a statistically significant decrease in total score (p=0.014). Boys prescore was 4.9±2.9 and postscore was 7.1±1.9 in the intervention group (p<0.001). Girls prescore was 5.7±2.6 and postscore was 7.1±2.4 in the intervention group (p<0.001). It is valuable to refer to the boys’ attitudes were at lower level in prescore compared to girls, but the postscore was at the same level. Boys achieved lower postscore in the control group at a statistically significant level (prescore 5.2±2.4 vs postscore 4.5±3.2, p=0.019). Girls achieved a lower postscore (6.3±2.7) in the control group (vs prescore 6.6±2.4) but not at a statistically significant level (p=0.264). In the 1st grade, the attitudes prescore and postscore were higher than in other grades. In all grades, pre and post attitudes scores were statistically significant (p<0.001) (Table 2). In 1st grade pre and post attitudes scores were 5.5±2.7 and 7.3±2.3, in 2nd grade they were 4.9±3.1 and 6.7±2.0, and in 3rd grade they were 4.6±2.4 and 6.8±1.7, in the intervention group, respectively. The scores of the control group were 6.4±2.3 and 5.6±3.2 for 1st grade (p<0.001), for 2nd grade they were 5.7±2.9 and 5.9±2.9 (p=0.521) and for 3rd grade they were 5.7±2.5 and 5.3±3.1 (p=0.177), respectively.

Obviously, taking into account the above results, we observed a decrease in intention to smoke in the intervention group. A significant increase in the percentage of students reporting that they were unlikely to smoke in the year following in the intervention group (94.4% vs 81.2%, p<0.001) was observed. It is remarkable to report the statistically significant (p<0.001) decrease of the negative intension to smoke in the future at the control group (82.4% vs 75.3%).

In Table 3, we delineate the results of the linear regression model, examining the effect of the intervention on knowledge and attitudes scores. These were adjusted according to baseline scores, school grade (as a proxy for age), and gender. All independent variables remained in both models due to the medical impact and despite the obvious no statistically significant impact of gender on knowledge score and the grade additionally to the gender on the attitudes score. We found that the intervention also had a significant effect on knowledge acquisition and attitude change in the adjusted, multi-variable regression model. The knowledge difference score was higher in grade 3 students compared to grade 1 students by 0.4722 units (p=0.012). In the control group the knowledge difference score was lower compared to the intervention group by 1.9681 units (p<0.001). For each unit of increase in pre knowledge score, the difference decreases by 0.6392 units (p<0.001). These results are referred at the baseline of 5.6068 units of knowledge difference score, taking into account all included parameters as independent variables. Similarly, the attitudes difference score was lower in the control group compared to the intervention group by 1.9212 units (p<0.001). For each unit of increase in pre attitudes score, the difference decreases by 0.5086 units (p<0.001). These results are referred at the baseline of 4.3335 units of attitudes difference score taking into account all the model parameters.

Table 3

Predictors of the difference between total post- and pre- intervention scores in knowledge and attitudes of 351 students in Athens

Beta coefficientS.E.tp>t95% CI
Knowledge difference score
Grade0.22420.09162.450.0150.0440.404
Gender-0.00140.1478-0.010.993-0.2920.289
Intervention-1.97120.1563-12.61<0.001-2.279-1.664
Baseline score-0.63770.0376-16.96<0.001-0.712-0.564
Constant7.33480.315723.24<0.0016.7147.956
Knowledge difference score (Explanation model)
Grade 20.11190.19570.570.568-0.2730.497
Grade 30.47220.18702.530.0120.1040.840
Female0.00760.14850.050.959-0.2850.300
Control group-1.96810.1565-12.57<0.001-2.276-1.660
Baseline score-0.63920.0377-16.95<0.001-0.713-0.565
Constant5.60680.254622.02<0.0015.1066.108
Attitudes difference score
Grade-0.01480.1540-0.100.924-0.3180.288
Gender0.31650.25201.260.210-0.1790.812
Intervention-1.91700.2568-7.46<0.001-2.422-1.412
Baseline score-0.51050.0478-10.69<0.001-0.604-0.417
Constant6.29080.483113.02<0.0015.3417.241
Attitudes difference score (Explanation model)
Grade 20.16230.32810.490.621-0.4830.808
Grade 3-0.06580.3138-0.210.834-0.6830.552
Female0.30030.25361.180.237-0.1990.799
Control group-1.92120.2571-7.47<0.001-2.427-1.415
Baseline score-0.50860.0479-10.62<0.001-0.603-0.414
Constant4.33350.328713.18<0.0013.6874.980

DISCUSSION

Our intervention was experienced and mastered by students who shared it with their classmates, understanding and describing its meaning. They identified, discussed and analyzed the key points through reflection, decoding of the symbols and their projection in similar situations. Then they generalized and related the experience and skills they acquired with everyday life, connecting them with examples from real life. Finally, they applied what they learned in similar situations20.

Our intervention was effective among adolescents aged 12–15 years, a vulnerable and critical period for tobacco experimentation and uptake. Students in the intervention group were more likely to improve their awareness of the effects of smoking, adopt attitudes against smoking and less likely to smoke in the first year following the intervention.

Increased awareness has been associated with a decrease in motivation to smoke and a lower probability of tobacco uptake. In contrast, a lack of awareness may lead to complacency and carelessness for unhealthy smoking behavior27. Our study findings resulted in significantly enhanced knowledge of the direct, imminent, and tangible consequences of smoking and are in line with other international reports demonstrating that school-based programs are efficient in knowledge acquisition28. This program aimed at understanding both the adverse effects of intermittent tobacco use and passive (secondhand and thirdhand) smoking. Intermittent tobacco use can lead to the development of regular smoking in adulthood29, while comprehension of the dangers of passive smoking can protect teenagers from unnecessary exposure, prevent habitual tobacco use and encourage individuals to respect no-smoking rules at home3.

Negative attitudes towards tobacco significantly improved at the follow-up at 3 months among the intervention group. Improvement was observed in questions relating to attitudes about the perceived benefits of and social stigma surrounding smoking among teenagers (e.g. acceptance and recognition from peers). A positive attitude toward tobacco use has been shown to correspond to a high intention to smoke30, an increased frequency of tobacco uptake, and a continuation of smoking into adulthood31. In line with relevant studies, our experiential learning program was effective in increasing the percentage of participants who reported intention to smoke as ‘unlikely’ at follow-up. There was also a significant reduction in students reporting intention to smoke as ‘very likely’, which may translate to a decreased chance of actual future tobacco uptake, according to the Theory of Planned Behavior32.

Limitations

Limitations of the present study include the use of a subjective, self-reported questionnaire, and a lack of cross-examination using an objective laboratory biomarker, such as cotinine. Notwithstanding, low metabolite levels, coupled with an irregular pattern of tobacco use among adolescents, challenge the validity of biochemical markers. In this context, self-reported tools are considered an equally reliable method of analyzing smoking behavior33. To avoid performance bias, the experiential learning intervention was carried out by the same investigator in all class sessions. Further, the effectiveness of our intervention was recorded at follow-up at 3 months, which could raise questions about its long-term efficacy. School-based studies with a prolonged follow-up are generally difficult and impractical, given limitations of the academic calendar. Nevertheless, existing literature suggests that effective short-term, school-based programs may have potential lasting effects34.

Additionally, although the student participants were enrolled from a ‘convenient sample’, we believe that the selected student population is relatively representative of the students attending public schools in Athens because the student population of the public education system is more or less homogeneous with respect to the socioeconomic status and school performance.

Data collection was conducted only for students present on the day of the intervention and at follow-up. Per wider literature, absent students tend to demonstrate lower academic achievement and appear more susceptible to addictive behaviors, potentially skewing study results35. In the present study there was no significant difference in absence between the two groups. Finally, the mean age of students in the intervention group was slightly lower than that of the control group, which may have influenced the prevalence of smoking and students’ receptiveness to anti-smoking messages. That said, the proportion of student smokers in our study was similar in both groups. Additionally, we found that the effect of the intervention was greater in the 2nd grade which was represented almost equally in both groups.

CONCLUSIONS

The findings of our study support our initial hypothesis; school-based, experiential learning smoking prevention programs are effective in augmenting smoking-related knowledge, enhancing anti-smoking attitudes, and minimizing intention to initiate or continue smoking. All outcome measures demonstrated a significant increase in the intervention group at three months post-implementation. Group differences can be attributed to the intervention itself and agree with similar studies, most of which support the efficacy and superiority of newer experiential learning programs. The current study is the first to utilize an interdisciplinary approach and use ancient Greek literature, myths and poems as educational tools.

Future research is needed to explore the effects of such a program in larger adolescent samples over an extended follow-up period, and further, explore its utility through parental involvement and the use of mass media. Besides, appraisal of the current program in settings outside formal education may expand its scope. Consideration of the above parameters will reinforce the current findings, augment our understanding of adolescent smoking patterns and encourage the design of future school-based interventions. Experiential smoking prevention programs may constitute a successful strategy in preventing adolescent tobacco uptake and form a cornerstone of public health policies against the tobacco epidemic.